According to Cloward – Ohlin,
(1960:153), criminal sub-culture develops from the lower class
neighbourhoods where the successful criminal provide juvenile the
opportunities of engaging in criminal acts and, subsequently, becoming
delinquents. The successful criminal in these neighbourhoods are visible
and often willing to associate with juveniles and teach them the act of
criminality. According to them, most often, juveniles in the
lower-class neighbourhood do not have access to conventional role models
refined to make them successful in life through legitimate channels.
The juveniles adopt drugs and alcohol as escape from life’s
difficulties.
Clinard and Abbott (1973:187) in their research on crime in developing countries found that the:
Poverty
of the lower classes prevents them from using legitimate means, such as
education, to acquire valued goods, thus giving them no alternative,
but to engage in illegitimate activities … the logical conclusion is
that opening up avenues of educational and economic opportunity will
reduce juvenile delinquency. The experience of economically developed
countries, however, has been the oppositeâ€.
According to
Albert Cohen (1955), the experiences of children and young persons,
including adolescents in the slum neighbourhoods where they are growing
up provides the ripe condition for delinquent subculture to develop.
This implies that a delinquent subculture of the slum area is a function
of socio-economic deprivations that juveniles suffer in the society.
The conditions of poverty creates a monster in some individuals who bind
together to form a gang of delinquent informed by a subculture of their
own (Cohen, 1955: 25 – 133).
There is a disproportionate amount
of official delinquent behaviour in slum neighbourhoods, which
represent a protest against the norms and values of the middle class
culture. Lower class youth’s become delinquents when they experience
culture conflict form their incapability to achieve success in
legitimate social sentences. Delinquency is a short-run,
non-utilitarian, malicious, and negativistic form of activity,
delinquents form gangs based on a deviant subculture which opposes the
value system of the larger society (Iwarimie – Jaja, 1999:5).
IQ levels and juvenile delinquency
An often cited 1977 study by Travis Hirschi and Michael Hindelang
revised interest in the association between IQ and delinquency. After
conducting a statistical analysis on a number of data, Hirchi and
Hindelang concluded both that IQ tests are a valid predictor of
intelligence and that weight of evidence is that IQ is more important
than race and social class for predicting delinquent involvement. They
argued that a low IQ increases the likelihood of delinquent behaviour
through its effect on school performance. Youth with low IQs do poorly
in school, and school failure and academic incompetence are highly
related to delinquency.
In 1926, William Healy and Augusta
Bronner tested a group of delinquents in Chicago and Boston and found
that 37% were subnormal in intelligence. They concluded that delinquents
were five to ten, times more likely to be mentally deficient than
non-delinquent boys.
These and other early studies were embraced
as proof that low IQ scores indicated potentially delinquent children
and that a correlation existed between innate low intelligence and
deviant behaviour.
Studies challenging the assumption that
people automatically committed delinquent acts because they had below –
average IQs began to appear as early as the 1920s. John Salwson studied,
1,543 delinquent boys in New York institutions and compared them with a
control group of New York City boys in 1926. He found that although 80%
of the delinquents were about normal in mechanical aptitude and
nonverbal intelligence. These results indicated the possibility of
cultural bias in portions of the IQ tests. He found also no relationship
between the numbers of arrests, the types of offence and the IQs.
Learning, Disability and Juvenile Delinquency
Moreover, researchers have shown a link between learning disability
(LD) and juvenile delinquency to buttress biological theory of juvenile
delinquency. Learning disabled kids usually exhibit poor motor
coordination (poor hand-eye coordination, trouble climbing stars, they
are clumsy, cannot catch a ball, cannot, stay neat), have behaviour
problems (lack emotional control, appear hostile, daydream a lot, and
improper auditory and vocal responses (do not seem to hear, cannot
differentiate sounds and noises).
The relationship between
learning disabilities and juvenile delinquency has been highlighted by
studies showing that arrested and incarcerated children have a far
higher LD rate than do children in the general population. While it is
estimated that approximately 10% of all youths have learning disorders
estimates of LD among adjudicated delinquents range from 26% to 73%.
Charles Murray (1976), writing in a widely read federally sponsored
study, offered two possible explanations of the link between learning
disability and delinquency. One view, known as the susceptibility
rationale, argues that the link is caused by certain side effects of
learning disabilities, such as impulsiveness, poor ability to learn from
experience, and inability to take social cues. In contrast, the school
failure rationale assumes that the frustration caused by the LD child’s
poor school performance will lead to a negative self-image and acting
out behaviour.